Main

For all cancers, stage at diagnosis is important for survival. For example, 5-year survival from colorectal cancer varies from 87% for tumours limited to the bowel wall (Dukes A) to 15% for advanced tumours in whom only palliative resection is undertaken (McArdle and Hole, 2002). There is concern that the relatively poor survival rates from cancer reported in the United Kingdom may be caused by relatively late stage disease at diagnosis and that this may reflect delays before treatment (Coebergh et al, 1998; Gatta et al, 2000). Within the United Kingdom, chances of both early diagnosis of and survival from cancer vary according to where people live. In Scotland, for several common cancers, increasing distance from cities with cancer centres is associated with more advanced disease at diagnosis and poorer early survival (Campbell et al, 2000, 2001). This accords with studies in Europe and the United States (Liff et al, 1991; Launoy et al, 1992).

The principle that all patients wherever they live should have access to a uniformly high quality of care to ensure the best possible survival rates was laid down by the Expert Advisory Group on Cancer in England and Wales and endorsed in Scotland (EAGC, 1995; SCCAC, 1996). Recognising the importance of stage at diagnosis, the cancer plans have set targets in an attempt to speed up time to treatment (NHS, 2000). Little is known, however, about factors that influence delay in diagnosis, nor how they affect delay at the various levels where it can occur. Delays in diagnosis can occur before presentation (patient delay) or between initial presentation and treatment (provider delay). This study aimed to determine for patients with breast and colorectal cancer if the time between first presentation and specialist treatment was longer for patients who lived further from cancer centres than for patients who lived nearby, and to discover which other patient, general practice and secondary care characteristics were associated with time to treatment.

Methods

People resident in the Grampian, Tayside or Highland Health Board areas of Scotland who were diagnosed with a first primary tumour of colorectal or breast cancer (female) between 1 January 1997 and 31 December 1998 were identified from the Scottish Cancer Registry (Brewster et al, 1997). Their names, dates of birth and death, sex, postcodes and health boards of residence at time of diagnosis, cancer sites, practices registered with at time of diagnosis, incidence dates and histories of previous cancers were obtained from the registry. Information on dates of death had been provided by the General Register Office (Scotland) and linked with the cancer registration record by record linkage at the Information and Statistics Division of the NHS in Scotland (Kendrick and Clarke, 1993). Cases that were duplicates, or could not be matched to 1991 census output areas, or were not allocated to practices in the study areas were excluded.

Permission was requested from all general practitioners in the study area with eligible patients to review medical records. Patients who had moved from practices were excluded from the study. Records of deceased patients were requested from the Practitioner Services Division of the NHS in Scotland. Data collected from medical records between May 2001 and May 2002 included presenting symptoms and clinical signs, dates and places of consultations, referral route and destination, date of first treatment (surgery, radiotherapy, chemotherapy or hormone therapy) and previous history (cancer, breast/bowel comorbidity, breast/bowel investigations with benign results, anxiety, depression, iron medication and smoking status).

A ‘2-year rule’ was used to reduce the likelihood of recording symptoms unrelated to the diagnosis of cancer. Working back in time from the date the cancer was diagnosed, the date was recorded of the first entry in the GP consultation notes of a relevant sign or symptom that appeared more than 2 years after any previous such sign or symptom. The list of eligible symptoms and signs was collated mainly from SIGN guidelines (SIGN, 1998, 2003).

Aggregated data from the 1991 Census were used to calculate distances to cancer centres and deprivation categories (each containing a fifth of the population of Grampian, Tayside and Highland) at the smallest level possible, the output area (Campbell et al, 2000, 2001). These were matched to postcodes of residence. Information on practice list sizes, and number and sexes of partners in a practice was obtained from an Information and Statistics Division database of Scottish general practitioners. Finally, information about the type of out of hours provision and waiting times for patients to get an appointment with the doctor of their choice was obtained from staff at practices visited.

Data were managed using Microsoft Access. Descriptive analyses were conducted using SPSS for Windows release 11 and multilevel modelling using MlwiN (Leyland and Goldstein, 2001). Data for breast and colorectal cancer were analysed separately. Our outcome was time from first presentation with suspicious symptoms or signs to first treatment. Our main independent variable (and the subject of our a priori hypothesis) was distance to a cancer centre, which was measured as the straight-line distance from the grid reference of output area of residence centroid (from Census data) to the nearest cancer centre (from the postcode directory) (Campbell et al, 2000, 2001). We also investigated relationships with time to treatment for multiple symptoms and variables – these analyses should be considered exploratory with statistical significance indicated for P<0.002.

In initial univariate analyses, times between presentation and treatment were highly skewed and so were log transformed to achieve a Normal distribution and have been converted back to geometric means and 95% confidence intervals for presentation. This initial analysis has limitations, however, because it excludes patients who were not treated, and does not take account of other significant factors or the structure of the data in four levels – patient, general practice, hospital of first referral and health board. In further analysis, multilevel modelling was employed using binary outcomes for delay (>30 days for breast cancer and >90 days for colorectal cancer) to prevent any bias that might have occurred had those who were not treated been excluded. These cut points were defined a priori as close to median expected delays and so results are presented in terms of odds ratios for delay along with 95% confidence intervals. All variables were considered in the multiple logistic regression models as all were considered to be clinically important. Initially, variables with P<0.10 (univariately) were entered together and all other variables were added individually and assessed at the conventional 5% level. Finally, a stepwise procedure was performed and compared with the final model for differences. All final models were assessed for extra-binomial variation.

Our proposed sample size was 2000 (1000 each with breast and colorectal cancer). Allowing for an attrition rate of 10–20%, this was calculated to have 80% power to detect an absolute difference of 10% in the proportions of cases waiting more than 30 days (breast cancer) or 90 days (colorectal cancer) for specialist treatment at the 5% significance level in two group comparisons. Ethical approval for the study was sought and obtained from Grampian, Tayside and Highland Research Ethics Committees.

Results

There were 1358 eligible people with breast cancer and 1457 with colorectal cancer (Figure 1). Of 217 general practices with live patients in the study area, 196 (90%) responded and 187 (86%) agreed to case notes review. The 218 (8%) live patients from practices not agreeing were omitted from the study, along with 143 (5%) who were no longer on their original general practice list, 111 (4%) deceased patients whose notes were unobtainable, and 23 (<1%) for various administrative reasons (principally case notes unobtainable at practice visits). General Practitioner medical records were reviewed for 1097 (80.8%) breast cancer cases of which 1069 had a time from presentation to treatment and 1223 (83.9%) colorectal cancer cases, of which 1071 had a time from presentation to treatment. The main reason for no time to treatment was death before treatment. There were few important differences in available characteristics of patients whose notes were and were not reviewed, but those not reviewed tended to have been younger (mean age 63 vs 67 years) and alive at the start of the study (79 vs 61%).

Figure 1
figure 1

Study profile.

Breast cancer

The geometric mean for time from presentation to treatment was 42 days (95% confidence interval 40–45). On univariate analysis, patients living further from cancer centres tended to be treated more quickly than those living nearby – the fastest time of 36 days (30–42) was for people between 38 and 57 km away (Table 1). Older patients were treated more quickly and several organisational variables appeared to be important, but deprivation was not a significant factor. Time to treatment varied significantly according to presenting symptoms – it was fastest for those with bone pain or ulcerating or fungating lesions and slowest for those with bloody nipple discharge (Table 2). Increasing numbers of symptoms at presentation predicted faster treatment, but asymptomatic patients (predominantly detected by screening) were also treated quickly. Treatment was slower for those with previous benign investigations or benign breast disease.

Table 1 Time to treatment of breast and colorectal cancer according to geographic, organisational and basic demographic variables
Table 2 Time from presentation to treatment for presenting breast cancer symptoms and other clinical variables

When variables were assessed in a multilevel model, variation was found at individual and hospital of first referral levels, but not general practice or health board (Table 3). The main factors associated with odds of treatment before 30 days were clinical, but the number of symptoms that predicted faster treatment condensed to three – breast lump, change in skin contour and regional lymphadenopathy. Again, patients with no symptoms (detected by screening) were treated quickest, but excluding them, treatment was quicker for patients with more symptoms. Only one organisational variable remained significant: treatment was faster for patients referred to noncancer centre hospitals (other general hospitals and community hospital outreach clinics) than for those referred to cancer centres and fastest at private hospitals.

Table 3 Results of multilevel modelling on treatment within 30 days (yes/no) with variation at patient, hospital of referral and health board level for people with breast cancer

Colorectal cancer

For colorectal cancer the geometric mean for time from presentation to treatment was 138 days (127–152). On univariate analysis, there were no differences in times from first presentation to treatment for patients living different distances from cancer centres (Table 1). Age was important, with fastest treatment for those in the 50–64 year age group and men were treated more quickly than women. Again, several organisational variables appeared to be important, but health board of residence and deprivation category were not. With regard to symptoms, treatment was fastest for those with abdominal guarding, and slowest for those without abdominal pain (Table 4). Increasing numbers of symptoms and frequency of GP consultations predicted faster treatment but it was slower for those with a history of anxiety, depression or benign bowel disease. Tumours of the transverse colon and splenic flexure were treated more quickly than other sites.

Table 4 Time from presentation to treatment for presenting colorectal cancer symptoms and other clinical variables

In the multivariate analysis, there was variation at patient, practice and health board levels. There was cross-classification between general practice and hospital levels (patients from individual practices were referred to different hospitals) so the hospital level was excluded from the model (hospital type was included at the health board level). Odds of treatment within 90 days were higher for those with palpable masses, abdominal pain or tenesmus, and who consulted more frequently (Table 5). Age remained significant, following a quadratic relationship, meaning treatment was slower for the old and young. People with a history of anxiety or depression were only half as likely to be treated within 90 days and those on iron therapy at presentation were more likely to be treated quickly. Treatment appeared to be slower for women and faster for patients registered at practices with higher numbers of female GPs, although these differences were not statistically significant.

Table 5 Results of multilevel modelling of subject level variables on treatment for colorectal cancer within 90 days (yes/no) with levels subject, practice and health board

Discussion

We found no evidence that people living further from cancer centres received slower treatment. For breast cancer they were, by being referred initially to local general or community hospitals, treated more quickly. Speed of treatment appears to be largely determined by features of the tumour and the symptoms and signs it generates, but age is important in both cancers, type of hospital of referral in breast cancer and general practice factors in colorectal cancer.

The study benefited from a high rate of case note retrieval and information from primary care, where the majority of cancers present. Case notes in primary care have the advantage of being contemporaneous records, but also have some limitations. It is possible that some symptoms were not recorded at time of presentation, but our analysis is likely to include the dominant symptoms and reasons why people consulted. We set strict rules and protocols for data collection to ensure consistency but must accept that, for colorectal cancer, it may be difficult to define the point at which cancer symptoms present in patients with pre-existing bowel disease. That we did not, after adjustments, find delays in treatment for this group suggests that any effect from this, if it occurred, was not substantial. Prospectively, we identified and collected data on as many variables as possible thought to affect delay. This had the advantage of ensuring that possible associations with geographical and organisational factors were not biased by caseload – and indeed, after adjusting for clinical factors, most of these associations disappeared – but the disadvantage is the possibility of some associations emerging by chance. While, then, we can have confidence in our findings regarding urban rural differences (our a priori hypothesis), our findings on other factors would benefit from confirmation in other research.

Our findings demonstrate that clinical factors at presentation – symptoms, signs, and tumour properties – are the most important factors in determining time taken before treatment. For breast cancer, our best fitting model for rapid treatment contained the usual sign of breast cancer (a lump), one of locally advanced disease (change in skin contour) and another of regional spread (axillary or supraclavicular lymphadenopathy). The time taken to treat colorectal cancer was usually considerably longer than that for breast cancer – average times to treatment of nearly 5 months are worrying and could plausibly affect survival (Richards et al, 1999). Delays were associated particularly with the presence or absence of certain symptoms (e.g. a palpable mass, abdominal pain or tenesmus) and frequency of consultation. Tumours of the transverse colon and splenic flexure were treated more quickly, perhaps because of a high proportion of splenic flexure tumours presenting with obstruction (Mulcahy and O'Donoghue, 1997).

Several other factors we found to be related to delay may be concerned with medical perception of risk. Older people, whose risk of cancer is greater, were treated more quickly than the under 50s, whose symptoms are less likely to represent cancer (Selvachandran et al, 2002). In colorectal cancer, the odds of delayed diagnosis were doubled by a past history of anxiety and depression, perhaps because benign causes of gastrointestinal symptoms are thought common in this group so, when presenting with nonspecific symptoms, their risk of cancer is perceived to be low. Men with colorectal cancer were treated more quickly than women, but this difference appears to be largely explained by other factors because it was no longer significant in the multivariate model. Faster treatment for those on iron at presentation is odd. Given our protocols for data collection, this group of patients had not been anaemic for 2 years, so are likely to have a longstanding history of iron deficiency. Perhaps, their risk of colorectal cancer with a given presentation was perceived to be greater – or perhaps this was a chance finding.

Although average times to treatment of colorectal cancer were much longer than breast, they were similar when both presented with a palpable mass. This suggests that much of the delay in colorectal cancer is related to the nonspecific nature of its early symptoms and signs (Selvachandran et al, 2002; Tan et al, 2002). First, there may be reluctance to expose these patients to investigations that carry a small but significant risk (SIGN, 2003). Second, limited investigative resources in our health care system mean that rapid investigation and treatment are often only available for selected patients – deemed ‘urgent’ – and selection is clearly difficult. In line with this, treatment was faster in the private sector, where investigations are more readily available – although the average time saving compared with cancer centres or general hospitals was only about 3 weeks.

Adjusting for the above factors, we found few associations with geographical, socioeconomic or organisational variables. At the population level, neither rural residence nor socioeconomic deprivation were associated with delays in treatment, so provider delays do not explain the higher chance of advanced disease reported for these groups of patients (Macleod et al, 2000; Campbell et al, 2001). Alternative explanations should be sought, including the time taken by patients before presentation (Bain and Campbell, 2000).

Three organisational factors appeared, however, to be important. First, breast cancer was treated most quickly if detected by screening. This appears to show the benefits of a well-organised, systematic approach to detection and treatment. More than 80% of people with breast cancer presented outside the screening programme in our study, and this group might benefit from a similarly efficient management pathway. Second, adjusting for clinical factors, treatment delays were more likely for women with breast cancer referred to cancer centres than other hospitals. This problem with centralised care has been reported for other cancers (Milne et al, 2000), but must be balanced against the possible benefits of specialist treatment (Mikeljevic et al, 2003). Finally, time to treatment of colorectal cancer varied among general practices, but this variation was not adequately explained by the factors we considered. There was a suggestion that treatment may be faster in practices with more female general practitioners, but no evidence of systematic differences between practices of different size or for those with longer waiting times for appointments.

In conclusion, the time taken to treat breast and colorectal cancers appears to be due largely to characteristics of the tumour and how it presents. Long delays are common for colorectal cancer, but we found that time to treatment was no greater for people with many factors previously suspected to cause delay – including geographical factors, deprivation and many general practice factors.